Commentary on ‘Reproducible brain-wide association studies require thousands of individuals’

Other

Commentary on ‘Reproducible brain-wide association studies require thousands of individuals’

Structural MRIFunctional MRI

Abstract

Received

Correspondence valk@cbs.mpg.de; Meike.Hettwer@maxplanckschools.de

A primary assumption of cognitive neurosciences is that brain structure relates to its function and consequently to behavior. Indeed, inquiries assessing genetic relationships between genes and behavior indicate a genetic link, suggesting a shared biological basis (1, 2). At the same time, there are various factors influencing measures of inter- and intra-individual variation in brain structure, function, and behavior. For example, local brain structure and function are not stable features but change across the lifespan (3, 4) and vary as a function of contextual factors, as well as covariates, such as fluid intake (5), time of day (6), blood pressure (7), and sex (8, 9). Moreover, also behavior varies across the lifespan (10) and is modulated, or confounded, by various factors, such as mood (11), social factors (12), and/or time of day (13). In addition, task-based and questionnaire-based markers of the same behavior do not always align (14). Thus, we are left with (i) variable brain metrics, (ii) variable behavioral metrics, mostly measured at a single timepoint in a lot of individuals, and (iii) covariates that may moderate brain–behavior associations.

So, what is the take-home from this? Should any inquiry into the association between covariation of brain and behavior be abolished? We believe not. In our opinion, the work of Marek et al. actually points to an interesting challenge, namely, how to understand individual cognition in the scope of an ever-changing brain structure and function. First, even if brain–behavior associations are not reproducible, it is relevant to understand why different subgroups show variable associations between brain and behavior. For example, brain structure has been reported to be differentially linked to fluid intelligence as a function of age (15), and brain function in females may be differentially associated with emotion regulation as a function of the menstrual cycle phase (16). Second, we believe that the question of how brain structure and function relate to behavior needs to go hand in hand with theories on what marker(s) of brain structure and function (area, cortical thickness, volume, in vivo myelin/microstructure, deep white matter, areas, gradients, networks, dynamics) may be relevant for what (kind of) behavioral process and respective functional involvement of which part(s) of the brain. So far, only a little research is done into what dimensions, or substrates, may be relevant for specific types of behavioral variation. Acquiring multimodal brain data, including proxies of microstructure, EEG, MEG, and high-resolution functional MRI (fMRI), in a small number of individuals using carefully selected behavioral metrics may provide novel mechanistic insights that go beyond associations based on correlating markers of ‘cognition’ with single indices of local brain structure in large samples.

In sum, we believe not all is lost, but this may be a great opportunity to rethink what we are looking for in the brain and to develop a mechanistic understanding of inter- and intra-individual variation in the brain and behavior. Preregistration of studies, open science, and multimodal acquisitions may help with these endeavors to develop a brain-informed understanding of human cognition in health and disease.

REFERENCES

1. Grasby, K.L., Jahanshad, N., Painter, J., Colodro-Conde, L., Bralten, J., Hibar, D.P. et al. The genetic architecture of the human cerebral cortex. Science 2020;367:6484.

2. Valk, S.L., Hoffstaedter, F., Camilleri, J.A., Kochunov, P., Yeo, P.T.T., Eickhoff, S.B. Personality and local brain structure: Their shared genetic basis and reproducibility. NeuroImage 2020;220:117067.

3. Bethlehem, R.A.I., Seidlitz, J., White, S.R., Vogel, J.W., Anderson, K.M., Adamson, C. et al. Brain charts for the human lifespan. Nature 2022;604:525–533.

4. Yates, T.S., Ellis, C.T., Turk-Browne, N.B. Emergence and organization of adult brain function throughout child development. NeuroImage 2021;226:117606.

5. Kempton, M.J., Ettinger, U., Foster, R., Williams, S.C.R., Calvert, G.A., Hampshire, A. et al. Dehydration affects brain structure and function in healthy adolescents. Hum Brain Mapp 2011;32:71–79.

6. Trefler, A., Sadeghi, N., Thomas, A.G., Pierpaoli, C., Baker, C.I., Thomas, C. Impact of time-of-day on brain morphometric measures derived from T1-weighted magnetic resonance imaging. NeuroImage 2016;133:41–52.

7. Schaare, H.L., Kharabian Masouleh, S., Beyer, F., Kumral, D., Uhlig, M., Reinelt, J.D. et al. Association of peripheral blood pressure with gray matter volume in 19- to 40-year-old adults. Neurology 2019;92:e758–e773.

8. Teeuw, J., Brouwer, R.M., Guimaraes, J.P.O.F.T., Brandner, P., Koenis, M.M.G., Swagerman, S.C. et al. Genetic and environmental influences on functional connectivity within and between canonical cortical resting-state networks throughout adolescent development in boys and girls. NeuroImage 2019;202:116073.

9. Liu, S., Seidlitz, J., Blumenthal, J.D., Clasen, L.S., Raznahan, A. Integrative structural, functional, and transcriptomic analyses of sex-biased brain organization in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci 2020;117:18788–18798.

10. Lindenberger, U. Lifspan theories of cognitive development. In: International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, Elsevier, 2001:13-8848.

11. Forgas, J.P. Emotions and Affect in Human Factors and Human-Computer Interaction, Editor: Myounghoon Jeon. Academic Press, 2017. Available from: http://usd-apps.usd.edu/coglab/schieber/hedonomics/pdf/Jeon2017-Chap1.pdf

12. Hein, G., Silani, G., Preuschoff, K., Batson, C.D., Singer, T. Neural responses to ingroup and outgroup members’ suffering predict individual differences in costly helping. Neuron 2010;68:149–160.

13. Goldstein, D., Hahn, C.S., Hasher, L., Wiprzycka, U.J., Zelazo, P.D. Time of day, intellectual performance, and behavioral problems in morning versus evening type adolescents: Is there a synchrony effect? Pers Individ Dif 2007;42:431–440.

14. Friedman, N.P., Banich, M.T. Questionnaires and task-based measures assess different aspects of self-regulation: Both are needed. Proc Natl Acad Sci 2019;116:24396–24397.

15. Kievit, R.A., Davis, S.W., Mitchell, D.J., Taylor, J.R., Duncan, J., Henson, R.N.A. et al. Distinct aspects of frontal lobe structure mediate age-
related differences in fluid intelligence and multitasking. Nat Commun 2014;5:5658.

16. Lusk, B.R., Carr, A.R., Ranson, V.A., Felmingham, K.L. Women in the midluteal phase of the menstrual cycle have difficulty suppressing the processing of negative emotional stimuli: An event-related potential study. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 2017;17:886–903.

This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (CC BY 4.0), which permits authors to copy and redistribute the material in any medium or format, remix, transform and build upon material, for any purpose, even commercially.